"Genius in Chess" by Jonathan Levitt, 1997 American Batsford Chess Library, Figurine Algebraic Notation, paperback, 128pp., $19.50 Reviewed by Glenn Budzinski Did you ever wonder, even if for only a moment, whether you had enough raw talent to make it to the top of the chess world someday? Does one really have to be a genius to be World Champion? British GM Jonathan Levitt's "Genius in Chess" purports to help you determine whether or not you have the right stuff. The book consists of 128 pages segregated into four parts, plus a preface, a forward and an index. Part 1, entitled "The Nature of Chess Genius", which corresponds to about one- quarter of the book, is a rather detailed discussion of the correlation between chess talent and genius, and the influence of IQ. Part 2 includes 8 separate tests (plus solutions) that can be taken to determine, to some degree, the extent of one's chess talent. A 20-question, multiple choice test (with answers at the end of the section), consisting mostly of middle and endgame positions, can be found in Part III, while the final part offers a few pages of advice on how to develop one's talent. The elements of genius, talent and intelligence are interwoven throughout "The Nature of Chess Genius" narrative section. Although there is much background information offered about how the game of chess is learned and understood, be forewarned: light reading, it is not. Levitt begins by distinguishing between the concepts of intelligence, creativity and genius, followed by a close look at what he believes it means to be a genius at chess. According to him, a chess genius is a person who exhibits certain traits such as a high IQ, a strong desire to win, one who is physically capable of performing his (or her) best at the board, superior concentration, perhaps a proper background (whatever that may mean), as well as unusually good perception and vision of the board and positions. Chess geniuses can be further segregated into two categories: the "Type A" genius, considered to be a positional or strategic player, like Steinitz, Capablanca and Karpov; and the "Type B" genius, such as Lasker, Alekhine, Tal and Kasparov, viewed as calculators or tacticians. (Fischer and Spassky are identified in a third category, "Universal", since they are, in Levitt's words, "harder to call," p. 33.) For Levitt, chess talent, or potential chess strength (which is different from current chess strength as determined by tournament play), is inextricably linked to IQ. In an attempt to quantify just how closely these two aspects are related, he came up with a mathematical formula called, not surprisingly, "The Levitt Equation". This formula calculates an Elo rating which expresses one's potential chess strength (talent) as being equal to ten times one's IQ, plus 1000 points. Thus, he claims that Bobby Fischer once scored 187 on an IQ test so, applying The Levitt Equation, Bobby's potential chess strength would be equal to a rating of about 2870 if he reached his peak talent. (187 x 10 = 1870 + 1000 = 2870) Of course, Levitt admits that there are certain difficulties associated with attempting to assign a strict numerical value to something as imprecise as chess talent. As a result, his subsequent tests will not enable one's "chess genius quotient" (chess talent) to be measured. According to him, "...the ability to score points in any test is not the only factor involved in chess success, so any such measure would be inherently unreliable and perhaps very misleading" (p. 43). Thus, when taking the Talent Tests of Part 2, all that one can do to determine whether one has some talent, is to compare the total time taken and number of correct responses on each test against the results given for a handful of mostly British players who, we are told, are either strong masters or titled players (i.e., GMs like Short, Sadler, Hodgson and Hertneck), and a few others. As previously mentioned, no method is provided to arrive at an overall score of one's chess talent. There are eight tests contained in Part 2: Count the Moves; Knight moves; Control and Mobility; King's Square Decision; Mate in One; Draw Tests; Analysis Test. A typical example of the almost maddening nature of some of these tests can be seen in the following Count the Moves test. Set up the following position: White: Ke4, Qa3, Rd2, Rf3, Nc6, Ne3, Be1, Bb3; pawns - a2, a4, a5, f2, f4, g3 Black: Kc3, Rc5,Rg4, Bb7; pawn - a6 To take this test, count the total number of moves for White and of that total, the number of moves that are mate in one. "Time your performance...and to encourage accuracy, add ten seconds to your time for each unit that you are out by, should you not have counted correctly..." explains Levitt (p. 46). For whatever it's worth, GMs Hodgson and Short, along with several masters, correctly answered both parts of the question; GMs Sadler and Hertneck did not. The latter, in fact, missed the mate in ones by a considerable margin. (For those of you who cared to try this problem, White has 29 moves and each of them is a mate in one.) Although there is no discussion of the scoring for the Multiple Choice Test in Part 3, Levitt states in the preface to that section that "I should tell you that I have shown some of the trickiest of these multiple choice questions to some very strong players (including grandmasters). They did not do at all well and continually fell into the traps. Number four seems to have thrown everybody, including even Michael Adams (for ten minutes or so)" (p. 81). For your amusement (or distress, whichever may be the most appropriate), here is question #4. Set up the following position: White: Ka2, Bc2; pawns - c7, d2, f5, g5, h5 Black: Kg7, Qd7, Rf8, Bh8, Ne1; pawns - b6, e6, f7 White is to move and has just been offered a draw by Black. Should he: A) Jump at the draw offer since he's down a veritable ton of material; B) Decline the draw and play the combination he had in mind, or C) Think about it but take the draw anyway, since it isn't worth taking the risk? (There is also a fourth choice offered that is clearly not a viable option.) The answer is almost as complicated as setting up the position: it's C), because after 1 h6+ Kg8 2 h7+ Kxh7 3 fxe6+ Qd3 4 Bxd3+ Kg8 5 e7 Ra8+ 6 Kb1 Be5 7 Be4 Re8 8 Bc6 Bxc7 9 Bxe8 Bd6 10 Ba4 Bxe7 11 Bxd1 Bxg5 12 Kc2, "The position is drawn, though it is simpler for White to accept the original offer rather than prove that the d-pawn gives sufficient counterplay in this final position", notes Levitt (p. 104).This is a book fraught with controversy and contradictions. Trouble begins early on and doesn't end until the reader reaches Part 4, the book's final, and probably best, six pages. Other than trying to impress us that he's done his homework researching the nature of intelligence and IQ testing and their relationship to chess, Levitt's theme for his 30-plus page, rambling narrative in Part 1 is unclear. For instance, he goes to great lengths to stress the inadequacies of IQ as a measuring device and dissuade the assignment of numerical values to talent tests, only to spend a disproportionate amount of time discussing IQ testing and suggesting a formula measuring chess talent based on, what else, but one's IQ. To label the Levitt Equation as controversial is hardly a profound statement, especially considering that the author admits such himself. This writer wishes to carry the controversy one step further by questioning the Equation's inclusion. Since the many mitigating factors make it impossible to quantify overall chess talent anyway, what is the point of the Levitt Equation, which is an approximate numerical rating of potential chess strength ? Isn't the author doing exactly what he said can't be done, because such would be unreliable and misleading ? Given the non-chess elements of the subject matter, a bibliography as well as a description of the author's professional qualifications would seem to be in order. Unfortunately, neither is provided. Even the tests are open to certain conjecture. Again, it was not surprising that two British problemists scored well on many of the problem-like positions, which would seem to beg the question of appropriateness of the tests. But the results themselves are difficult to assess, given the very small and narrow sample of players who took the tests and the various states of impairment of some of the subjects (pp. 61, 63, etc.). So, in the end, what does it mean if you didn't score as well as Nigel Short or Matthew Sadler ? Okay, so maybe you might not ever become a GM, but does that mean you just don't have what it takes to make master (assuming, of course, that any meaning at all can be attached to the results) ? And, how does one evaluate his performance on the multiple choice test, without a discussion of results? In fact, why bother to take the tests at all if you aren't able to interpret the results ? This writer took the tests--he didn't do as well as the GMs on the talent tests, correctly answered more than half of the multiple choice questions, and vaguely remembers that he was given an IQ test more than 30 years ago and scored somewhere in the 130s on it, meaning that his potential chess strength should be somewhere in the 2300s. Unfortunately, he has no more of an idea of the extent of his chess talent now, than he did before he read this book. There is, however, one thing that can be said with a degree of certainty: if chess talent can't be measured and if the tests don't mean anything, then why do we need this book?